The Surprising Upside of Dyslexia

Share Button

The word “dyslexia” evokes painful struggles with reading, and indeed this learning disability causes much difficulty for the estimated 15 percent of Americans affected by it. Since the phenomenon of “word blindness” was first documented more than a century ago, scientists have searched for the causes of dyslexia, and for therapies to treat it. In recent years, however, dyslexia research has taken a surprising turn: identifying the ways in which people with dyslexia have skills that are superior to those of typical readers. The latest findings on dyslexia are leading to a new way of looking at the condition: not just as an impediment, but as an advantage, especially in certain artistic and scientific fields.

Dyslexia is a complex disorder, and there is much that is still not understood about it. But a series of ingenious experiments have shown that many people with dyslexia possess distinctive perceptual abilities. For example, scientists have produced a growing body of evidence that people with the condition have sharper peripheral vision than others. Gadi Geiger and Jerome Lettvin, cognitive scientists at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology, used a mechanical shutter, called a tachistoscope, to briefly flash a row of letters extending from the center of a subject’s field of vision out to its perimeter. Typical readers identified the letters in the middle of the row with greater accuracy. Those with dyslexia triumphed, however, when asked to identify letters located in the row’s outer reaches.

Mr. Geiger and Mr. Lettvin’s findings, which have been confirmed in several subsequent studies, provide a striking demonstration of the fact that the brain separately processes information that streams from the central and the peripheral areas of the visual field. Moreover, these capacities appear to trade off: if you’re adept at focusing on details located in the center of the visual field, which is key to reading, you’re likely to be less proficient at recognizing features and patterns in the broad regions of the periphery.

The opposite is also the case. People with dyslexia, who have a bias in favor of the visual periphery, can rapidly take in a scene as a whole — what researchers call absorbing the “visual gist.”

Intriguing evidence that those with dyslexia process information from the visual periphery more quickly also comes from the study of “impossible figures,” like those sketched by the artist M. C. Escher. A focus on just one element of his complicated drawings can lead the viewer to believe that the picture represents a plausible physical arrangement.

A more capacious view that takes in the entire scene at once, however, reveals that Escher’s staircases really lead nowhere, that the water in his fountains is flowing up rather than down — that they are, in a word, impossible. Dr. Catya von Károlyi, an associate professor of psychology at the University of Wisconsin, Eau Claire, found that people with dyslexia identified simplified Escher-like pictures as impossible or possible in an average of 2.26 seconds; typical viewers tend to take a third longer. “The compelling implication of this finding,” wrote Dr. Von Károlyi and her co-authors in the journal Brain and Language, “is that dyslexia should not be characterized only by deficit, but also by talent.”

The discovery of such talents inevitably raises questions about whether these faculties translate into real-life skills. Although people with dyslexia are found in every profession, including law, medicine and science, observers have long noted that they populate fields like art and design in unusually high numbers. Five years ago, the Yale Center for Dyslexia and Creativity was founded to investigate and illuminate the strengths of those with dyslexia, while the seven-year-old Laboratory for Visual Learning, located within the Harvard-Smithsonian Center for Astrophysics, is exploring the advantages conferred by dyslexia in visually intensive branches of science. The director of the laboratory, the astrophysicist Matthew Schneps, notes that scientists in his line of work must make sense of enormous quantities of visual data and accurately detect patterns that signal the presence of entities like black holes.

A pair of experiments conducted by Mr. Schneps and his colleagues, published in the Bulletin of the American Astronomical Society in 2011, suggests that dyslexia may enhance the ability to carry out such tasks. In the first study, Mr. Schneps reported that when shown radio signatures — graphs of radio-wave emissions from outer space — astrophysicists with dyslexia at times outperformed their nondyslexic colleagues in identifying the distinctive characteristics of black holes.

In the second study, Mr. Schneps deliberately blurred a set of photographs, reducing high-frequency detail in a manner that made them resemble astronomical images. He then presented these pictures to groups of dyslexic and nondyslexic undergraduates. The students with dyslexia were able to learn and make use of the information in the images, while the typical readers failed to catch on.

Given that dyslexia is universally referred to as a “learning disability,” the latter experiment is especially remarkable: in some situations, it turns out, those with dyslexia are actually the superior learners.

Mr. Schneps’s study is not the only one of its kind. In 2006, James Howard Jr., a professor of psychology at the Catholic University of America, described in the journal Neuropsychologia an experiment in which participants were asked to pick out the letter T from a sea of L’s floating on a computer screen. Those with dyslexia learned to identify the letter more quickly.

Whatever special abilities dyslexia may bestow, difficulty with reading still imposes a handicap. Glib talk about appreciating dyslexia as a “gift” is unhelpful at best and patronizing at worst. But identifying the distinctive aptitudes of those with dyslexia will permit us to understand this condition more completely, and perhaps orient their education in a direction that not only remediates weaknesses, but builds on strengths.

Share Button

7 Responses to “The Surprising Upside of Dyslexia”

  1. My dyslexia has served me well my whole life. I’m hard wired for narrative. See and understand patterns. Describe concepts and ideas visually. Draw to explain concepts. Best of all I just published my first book on negotiation for creatives. A friend recommended this book by Brock and Fernette Eide to me a year ago on dyslexia: http://www.amazon.com/The-Dyslexic-Advantage-Unlocking-Potential/dp/0452297923

  2. My son has spent his life working through and helping others with Dyslexia. He is very visual and has spoken to over 500,000 kids, parents, and teachers. His website above is being used by State of Va. to teach teachers. It is a broadcast website and he has over hundred hours of training that isn’t even on it yet. On it he offers twenty years of talks and his two books on Dyslexia.
    He is very special in that he gets to change a life everyday.
    dyslexiaed.com

  3. Ira Socol says:

    Dyslexia, like ADHD and many other things diagnosed by “medical model” loving Americans, is neither good nor bad, simply another human variation, which, if current brain research is correct, describes the way about 2/3 of the global population reads. (Which might be why American education has always failed so badly – in analysis since 1867 no more than 34% of American students have ever gotten beyond the 6th grade reading level).

    As for the link on your site about “Making Activities” and dyslexia, well, sure. In our Maker Schools, where Maker activities and creation are part of all learning environments, all students, including dyslexic and ADHD students do better, but I’d never make the claim that dyslexia can be linked to skills in that field. “Dyslexia” is far too broad a definition to ever tie it clearly to other neural pathways.

    But remaking schools to engage the natural learning patterns of childhood and adolescence absolutely works. http://www.cvilletomorrow.org/news/article/17803-local-schools-turn-to-making-in-the-classroom/

  4. CatT says:

    A friend sent me the article that came from this blog. I must say that as a dyslexic, the daughter of a dyslexic, and a former lecturer in design, esp. 3D & Industrial design; I am not at all surprised at the upside! I noticed these & more myself & used to get interesting responses when discussing them openly at international conferences etc were I was presenting my research into design education. The only surprise for me was that non-dyslexics have bothered to take it seriously enough to look into them & even write & publish about it.
    I’ll keep writing with or without my dyslexia. After all, it was school & under grad that was tough, anything after that has been tailor-made for me in comparison :-)

  5. Dee Young says:

    What a wonderful article. As someone who has dyslexia and other learning and processing differences, I have always hoped it would stop being labeled as something so wrong/broken and thus needing to be “fixed.” The statement, “Those with dyslexia are actually the superior learners” is so true. Most people with processing or learning differences have had learn to adapt and function on a higher level to get along in this written world.

  6. Ron Teffaine says:

    I think suggesting that all people with dyslexia are gifted in seeing visual patterns or are all natural creative artists is misleading. Surely there are people with dyslexia who have these talents, but to suggest that if you have dyslexia, you also have these talents is misleading. That is not taking into consideration the individual differences people have.

Leave a Reply

Sign up for The Brilliant Report, a monthly newsletter full of the latest findings on how to learn smarter:

Close